英语六级 学英语,练听力,上听力课堂! 注册 登录
> 英语六级 > 六级阅读 >  内容

2019年12月大学英语六级阅读真题以及答案(一)

所属教程:六级阅读

浏览:

tingliketang

2024年08月19日

手机版
扫描二维码方便学习和分享

英语六级阅读真题,不仅强化词汇与句型理解,更提升阅读速度与综合分析能力。实战演练,让考生熟悉题型变化,掌握解题技巧,是冲刺六级高分不可或缺的宝贵资源。今天,小编将分享2019年12月大学英语六级阅读真题以及答案(卷一)相关内容,希望能为大家提供帮助!

Section A

Directions: In this section, there is a passage with ten blanks. You are required to select one word for each blank from a list of choices given in a word bank following the passage. Read the passage through carefully before making your choices. Each choice in the bank is identified by a letter. Please mark the corresponding letter for each item on Answer Sheet 2 with a single line through the centre. You may not use any of the words in the bank more than once.

When considering risk factors associated with serious chronic diseases, we often think about health indicators such as cholesterol, blood pressure, and body weight. But poor diet and physical inactivity also each increase the risk of heart disease and have a role to play in the development of some cancers. Perhaps worse, the  26  effects of an unhealthy diet and insufficient exercise are not limited to your body. Recent research has also shown that  27  in a high- fat and high- sugar diet may have negative effects on your brain, causing learning and memory  28  .

Studies have found obesity is associated with impairments in cognitive functioning, as  29  by a range of learning and memory tests, such as the ability to remember a list of words presented some minutes or hours earlier. There is also a growing body of evidence that diet- induced cognitive impairments can emerge  30  — within weeks or even days. For example, one study found healthy adults  31  to a high- fat diet for five days showed impaired attention, memory, and mood compared with a low- fat diet control group. Another study also found eating a high- fat and high- sugar breakfast each day for as little as four days resulted in problems with learning and memory  32  to those observed in overweight and obese individuals.

Body weight was not hugely different between the groups eating a healthy diet and those on high and sugar diets. So this shows negative  33  of poor dietary intake can occur even when body weight has not changed  34  . Thus, body weight is not always the best indicator of health and a thin person still needs to eat well and exercise  35  .

A) assessed

B) assigned

C) consequences

D) conspicuously

E) deficits

F) designated  

G) detrimental 

H) digestion

I) excelling

J) indulging

K) loopholes

L) rapidly 

M) redundant

N) regularly

O) similar

Section B

Directions: In this section, you are going to read a passage with ten statements attached to it. Each statement contains information given in one of the paragraphs. Identify the paragraph from which the information is derived. You may choose a paragraph more than once. Each paragraph is marked with a letter. Answer the questions by marking the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2.

Increased Screen Time and Wellbeing Decline in Youth

A) Have young people never had it so good? Or do they face more challenges than any previous generation? Our current era in the West is one of high wealth. This means minors enjoy material benefits and legal protections that would have been the envy of those living in the past. But There is an increasing suspicion that all is not well for our youth. And one of the most popular explanations, among some experts and the popular media, is that excessive“ screen time” is to blame.( This refers to all the attention young people devote to their phones, tablets and laptops.) However, this is a contentious theory and such claims have been treated skeptically by some scholars based on their reading of the relevant data.

B) Now a new study has provided another contribution to the debate, uncovering strong ev idence that adolescent wellbeing in the United States really is experiencing a decline and arguing that the most likely cause is the electronic riches we have given them. The background to this is that from the 1960s into the early 2000s, measures of average wellbeing went up in the US. This was especially true for younger people. It reflected the fact that these decades saw a climb in general standards of living and avoidance of mass societal traumas like full- scale war or economic deprivation. However, the“ screen time” hypothesis, advanced by researchers such as Jean Twenge, is that electronic devices and excessive time spent online may have reversed these trends in re cent years, causing problems for young people's psychological health.

C) To investigate, Twenge and her colleagues dived into the“ Monitoring the Future” databaset based on annual surveys of American school students from grades 8, 10, and 12 that started in 1991. In total, 1. I million young people answered various questions related to their wellbeing. Twenge's team's analysis of the answers confirmed the earlier, well- established wellbeing climb, with scores rising across the 1990s, and into the later2000s. This was found across measures like self- esteem, life satisfaction, happiness and satisfaction with individual domains like job, neighborhood, or friends. But around 2012 these measures started to decline. This continued through 2016, the most recent year for which data is available.

D) Twenge and her colleagues wanted to understand why this change in average wellbeing occurred.However, it is very hard to demonstrate causes using non- experimental data such as this. In fact, when Twenge previously used this data to suggest a screen time effect, some commentators were quick to raise this problem. They argued that her causal- sounding claims rested on correlational data, and that she had not adequately accounted for other potential causal factors. This time around, Twenge and her team make a point of saying that they are not trying to establish causes as such, but that they are assessing the plausibility of potential causes.

E) First, they explain that if a given variable is playing a role in affecting wellbeing, then we should expect any change in that variable to correlate with the observed changes in wellbeing. If not, it is not plausible that the variable is a causal factor. So the researchers looked at time spent in a number of activities that could plausibly be driving the wellbeing decline. Less sport, and fewer meetings with peers correlated with lower wellbeing, as did less time reading print media ( newspapers) and, surprisingly, less time doing homework.( This last find ing would appear to contradict another popular hypothesis that it is our burdening of students with assignment that is causing all the problems.) In addition, more TV watching and more electronic com munication both correlated with lower wellbeing. All these effects held true for measures of happiness, life satisfaction and self- esteem, with the effects stronger in the 8th and 10th- graders

F) Next, Twenge's team dug a little deeper into the data on screen time. They found that adolescents who spent a very small amount of time on digital devices—a couple of hours a week— had the highest wellbeing. Their wellbeing was even higher than those who never used such devices. However, higher doses of screen time were clearly associated with lower happiness. Those spending 10-19 hours per week on their devices were 41 percent more likely to be unhappy than lower- frequency users. Those who used such devices 40 hours a week or more( one in ten teenagers) were twice as likely to be unhappy. The data was slightly complicated by the fact that there was a tendeney for kids who were social in the real world to also use more online communication, but by bracketing out different cases it became clear that the real- world sociality compone nt correlated with greater wellbeing, whereas greater time on screens or online only correlated with poorer wellbeing.

G) So far, so plausible. But the next question is, are the drops in average wellbeing happening at the same time as trends toward increased electronic device usage? It looks like it — after all, 2012 was the tipp ing point when more than half of Americans began owning smartphones. Twenge and her colleagues also found that across the key years of 2013-16, wellbeing was indeed lowest in years where adolescents spent more time online, on social media, and reading news online, and when more youth in the United States had smartphones. And in a second amalysis, they found that where technology went, dips in wellbeing followed. For instance, years with a larger increase in online usage were followed by years with lower wellbeing, rather than the other way around. This does not prove causality, but is consistent with it. Meanwhile, TV use did not show this tracking. TV might make you less happy, but this is not what seems to be driving the recent declines in young people's average happiness.

H)A similar but reversed pattern was found for the activities associated with greater wellbeing. For example,years when people spent more time with friends were better years for wellbeing ( and followed by better years). Sadly, the data also showed face- to- face socializing and sports activity had declined over the period covered by the survey.

I) There is another explanation that Twenge and her colleagues wanted to address: the impact of the great recession of 2007-2009, which hit a great number of American families and might be affecting adolescents. The data set they used did not include economic data, so instead the researchers looked at whether the 2013-16 wellbeing decline was tracking economic indicators. They found some evidence that some crude measures, like income inequality, correlated with changes in wellbeing, but economic measures with a more direct impact, like family income and unemployment rates( which put families into difficulties) , had no relationship with wellbeing. The researchers also note the recession hit some years before we see the beginning of the wellbeing drop, and before the steepest wellbeing decline, which occurred in 2013.

J) The researchers conclude that electronic communication was the only adolescent activity that increased at the same time psychological wellbeing declined . I suspect that some experts in the field will be keen to address alternative explanations, such as unassessed variables playing a role in the well being decline. But the new work does go further than previous research and suggests that screen time should still be considered a potential barrier to young people's flourishing.

36. The year when most Americans began using smartphones was identified as a turning point in young Americans' level of happiness.

37. Scores in various wellbeing measures began to go downward among young Americans in recent years.

38. Unfortunately, activities involving direct contact with people, which contributed to better wellbeing were found to be on the decline.

39. In response to past critics, Twenge and her co- researchers stress they are not trying to prove that the use of digital devices reduces young people's wellbeing.

40. In the last few decades of the 20th century, living standards went up and economic depressions were largely averted in the US.

41. Contrary to popular belief, doing homework might add to students' wellbeing.

42. The author believes the researchers' new study has gone a step further regarding the impact of screen time on wellbeing.

43. The researchers found that extended screen time makes young people less happy.

44. Data reveals that economic inequality rather than family income might affect people's wellbeing.

45. Too much screen time is widely believed to be the cause of unhappiness among today's young people.

Section C

Directions: There are 2 passages in this section. Each passage is followed by some questions or unfinished statements. For each of them there are four choices marked A) , B), C) and D). You should decide on the best choice and mark the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2 with a single line through the centre.

Passage One

Questions 46 to 50 are based on the following passage.

“ The dangerous thing about lying is people don't understand how the act changes us,” says Dan Ariely, behavioural psychologist at Duke University . Psychologists have documented children lying as early as the age of two. Some experts even consider lying a developmental milestone, like crawling and walking, because it requires sophisticated planning, attention and the ability to see a situation from someone else's perspective to manipulate them. But, for most people, lying gets limited as we develop a sense of morality and the ability to self- regulate.

Harvard cognitive neuroscientist Joshua Greene says. for most of us, lying takes work. In studies, he gave subjects a chance to deceive for monetary gain while examining their brains in a functional MRI machine, which maps blood flow to active parts of the brain. Some people told the truth instantly and instinctively. But others opted to lie, and they showed increased activity in their frontal parietal(颅腔壁的) control network, which is involved in difficult or complex thinking. This suggests that they were deciding between truth and dishonesty— and ultimately opting for the latter. For a follow- up analysis, he found that people whose neural(神经的) reward centres were more active when they won money were also more likely to be among the group of liars— suggesting that lying may have to do with the inability to resist temptation.

External conditions also matter in terms of when and how often we lie. We are more likely to lie, research shows, when we are able to rationalise it, when we are stressed and fatigued or see others being dishonest. And we are less likely to lie when we have moral reminders or when we think others are watching.“ We as a society need to understand that, when we don't punish lying, we increase the probability it will happen again,” Ariely says.

In a 2016 study published in the journal Nature Neuroscience, Ariely and colleagues showed how dishonesty alters people's brains, making it easier to tell lies in the future. When people uttered a falsehood, the scientists noticed a burst of activity in their amygdala. The amygdala is a crucial part of the brain that produces fear, anxiety and emotional responses including that sinking, guilty feeling you get when you lie. But when scientists had their subject s play a game— in which they w on money by deceiving their partner, they noticed the negative signals from the amygdala began to decrease. Not only that, but when people faced no consequences for dishonesty, their falsehoods tended to get even more sensational . This means that if you give people multiple opportunities to lie for their own benefit, they start with little lies which get bigger over time.

46. Why do some experts consider lying a milestone in a child's development?

A) It shows they have the ability to view complex situations from different angles.

B) It indicates they have an ability more remarkable than crawling and walking.

C) It represents their ability to actively interact with people around them.

D) It involves the coordination of both their men tal and physical abilities.

47. Why does the Harvard neuroscientist say that lying takes work?

A) It is hard to choose from several options.  

B) It is difficult to sound natural or plausible.

C) It requires speedy blood flow into one's brain.  

D) It involves lots of sophisticated men tal activity.

48. Under what circumstances do people tend to lie?

A) When they become too emotional.  

B) When they face too much peer pressure.

C) When the temptation is too strong.  

D) When the consequences are not imminent.

49. When are people less likely to lie?

A) When they are wom out and stressed.  

B) When they are under watchful eyes.

C) When they think in a rational way.  

D) When they have a clear conscience.

50. What does the author say will happen when a liar does not get punished?

A) They may feel justified.  

B) They will tell bigger lies.

C) They will become complacent.

D) They may mix lies and truths.

Passage Two

Questions 51 to 55 are based on the following passage.

Here's how the Pacific Northwest is preparing for“ The Big One”. It's the mother of all disaster drills for what could be the worst disaster in American history. California has spent years preparing for“ The Big One”- the inevitable earthquake that will undoubtedly unleash all kinds of havoc along the famous San Andreas fault(断层). But what if the fault that runs along the Pacific Northwest delivers a gigantic earthquake of its own? If the people of the Cascadia region have anything to do with it, they won't be caught unawares.

The region is engaged in a multi- day earthquake- and- tsunami(海啸) drill involving around 20,000 people. The Cascadia Rising drill gives area residents and emergency responders a chance to practice what to do in case ofa9.0- magnitude earthquake and tsunami along one of the nation's dangerous— and underestimated— faults.

The Cascadia Earthquake Zone is big enough to compete with San Andreas( it's been called the most dangerous fault in America), but it's much lesser known than its Califomia cousin. Nearly 700 miles long, the earthquake zone is located by the North American Plate off the coast of Pacific British Columbia, Washington, Oregon and Northern California.

Cascadia is what's known as a“ megathrust” fault. Megathrusts are created in earthquake zones- land plate boundaries where two plates converge. In the areas where one plate is beneath another, stress builds up over time. During a megathrust event, all of that stress releases and some of the world's most powerful earthquake s occur. Remember the 9.1 earthquake and ts unam i in the Indian Ocean off Sumatra in 2004? It was caused by a megathrust event as the India plate moved beneath the Burma micro- plate.

The last time a major earthquake occurred along the Cascadia fault was in 1700, so officials worry that another event could occur any time. To prevent that event from becoming a catastrophe, first responders will join members of the public in rehearsals that involve communication, evacuation, search and rescue, and other scenarios.

Thousands of casualties are expected if a 9.0 carthquake were to occur. First, the earthquake would shake metropolitan areas including Seattle and Portland. This could trigger a tsunam i that would create havoc along the coast. Not all casualties can necessarily be prevented— but by coordinating across local, state, and even national borders, officials hope that the worst- case scenario can be averted. On the exercise's website, officials explain that the report they prepare during this rehearsal will inform disaster management for years to come.

For hundreds of thousands of Cascadia residents,“ The Big One” isn't a question of if, only when. And it's never too early to get ready for the inevitable.

51. What does“ The Big One” refer to?

A) A gigantic geological fault.  

B) A large- scale exercise to prepare for disasters.

C) A massive natural catastrophe.  

D) A huge tsunami on the California coast.

52. What is the purpose of the Cascadia Rising drill?

A) To prepare people for a major earthquake and tsunami.

B) To increase residents' awareness of imminent disasters.

C) To teach people how to adapt to post- disaster life.

D) To cope with the aftermath of a possible earthquake.

53. What happens in case of a megathrust earthquake according to the passage?

A) Two plates merge into one.  

B) A variety of forces converge.

C) Boundaries blur between plates.

D) Enormous stress is released.

54. What do the officials hope to achieve through the drills?

A) Coordinating various disaster- relief efforts.  

B) Reducing casualties in the event of a disaster.

C) Minimizing property loss caused by disasters.  

D) Establishing disaster and emergency management.

55. What does the author say about“ The Big One”?

A) Whether it will occur remains to be seen.  

B) How it will arrive is too early to predict.

C) Its occurrence is just a matter of time.

D) It keeps haunting Cascadia residents.

26. G) detrimental

语义判断:前文提到,糟糕的日常饮食和缺乏运动也会增加患上心脏疾病的风险, 而且还会对一些癌症的发展产生影响,而本句提到,不健康的饮食和运动量不足的……影响并不局限于你的身体,显然,增加患上心脏疾病的风险、对癌症的发展产生影响都属于“不利”影响。detrimental意为“不利的,有害的”符合语境,故本题应选G。

27. J) indulging

语义判断:本句提到,最近的研究还表明, ⋯⋯高脂肪、高糖的食物可能会对你的大脑产生负面影响,将符合语法条件的备选项代入原文,可知 indulgingin(沉溺于;肆意享受) 符合此处语境,故本题应选J。

28. E) deficits

语义判断:结合上面两题的分析可知,此处是介绍最近的研究发现。上文提到, 不健康的饮食和运动量不足的不利影响并不局限于你的身体。由此可知,本句是说,肆意吃高脂肪、高糖的食物可能会对你的大脑产生负面影响,导致学习和记忆“障碍”, deficits意为“不足, 缺乏”,可以引申为“障碍”,符合此处语境, 故本题应选E。

29. A) assessed

语义判断:本句为第二段开头句,前半句提到,研究发现,肥胖与认知功能受损有关,后半句使用 as引导的短语加以补充说明, 由 by a range of learning and memory tests(通过一系列学习和记忆测试)可知,符合语法条件的备选项中assessed符合语境,即“通过一系列学习和记忆测试评估”,在此处的语境中可以转译为“得出结论”,故本题应选A。

30. L) rapidly

语义判断:前半句提到,越来越多的证据也表明,饮食导致的认知障碍会……出现,后面使用破折号加以补充说明, 破折号后面指出 within weeks or even days(在几周甚至几天内),可见, 此处是说,饮食导致的认知障碍会“迅速”出现,rapidly符合语境, 故本题选L。

31. B) assigned

语义判断: 结合符合语法条件的备选项,由后面的compared with a low- fat diet control group(与低脂肪饮食的对照组相比)可知,此处是说,健康成年人“被指定”摄入高脂肪饮食五天后,就显示出注意力、记忆力和情绪受损的情况,assign sb. to sth.为固定搭配,意为“指定某人做某事”,符合语境,此处使用的是其过去分词结构 assigned to(被指定),故本题应选B。

32. O) similar

语义判断:前半句提到,另一项研究还发现,每天吃一顿高脂肪、高糖的早餐,只要四天,就会导致学习和记忆方面出现问题, 由后文to those observed in overweight and obese individuals(与观察到的超重和肥胖人群的情况)可知,连续四天吃高脂肪、高糖早餐的人产生的情况应与超重和肥胖人群的情况“类似”。similar to为固定搭配, 意为“类似于”, 符合语境,故本题选O。

33. C) consequences

语义判断:本段首句提到,健康饮食组和高脂肪、高糖饮食组的体重差异并不是特别大,而本句指出,即使体重没有……变化, 摄入不良饮食的负面……也会发生,将符合语法条件的备选项代入原文可知, 此处是指负面“影响”,consequences符合语境, 故本题应选C。

34. D) conspicuously

语义判断:上文提到,健康饮食组和高脂肪、高糖饮食组的体重差异并不是特别大,故此处应指,即使体重没有“显著”变化, 摄入不良饮食的负面影响也会产生,conspicuously意为“显著”,符合语境,故本题应选D。

35. N) regularly

语义判断:文章最后得出结论,因此,体重并不总是健康的最佳指标,身材消瘦的人仍然需要吃好,由needs to eat well and exercise可见, eat well和 exercise_____构成并列关系, 将 regularly代入原文,恰好符合这种并列关系,即“吃好,并且经常锻炼”,故本题应选N。

36.答案: G

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 The year when most Americans began using smartphones was identified as a turning point。文章段落中谈及大多数美国人开始使用智能手机的那一年被认为是转折点的内容出现在G段。该段探讨了平均幸福感降低是否与电子设备使用增多同时发生的问题,其中第三句提到,2012年是半数以上的美国人开始拥有智能手机的转折点。本句是对该段此句的同义转述,题干中The year指代原文中的2012; when most Americans began using smartphones对应原文中when more than half of Americans began owning smartphones; was identified as a turning point对应原文中的 was the tipping point.

37.答案: C

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息Scores in various wellbeing measures began to go downward。文章段落中谈及体现幸福感的各种指标下降的内容出现在C段。该段第三句指出, 这些指标的分数从20世纪90年代持续上升到21世纪00年代后期,特文格的团队从而得出结论:幸福感提升早年确实长期存在过。最后两句说, 2012年前后这些指标开始下降, 一直持续到2016年。其中 these measures回指上句中的 measures like self- esteem, life satisfaction, happiness and satisfaction with individual domains like job, neighborhood, or friends。本题题干是对C段内容的概括。题干中的 began to go downward是原文倒数第二句中 started to decline的同义转述; in recent years对应原文中的 around 2012及 continued through 2016。

38.答案: H

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 activities involving direct contact with people和 on the decline。文章段落中谈到与人直接接触的活动在减少的内容出现在H段。H段第二句谈到,人们花更多时间与朋友在一起的某几年中幸福感较高(接下来的几年幸福感更高)。说明与朋友相处这种直接与人接触的活动是可以提升幸福感的。但紧接着第三句又说,遗憾的是, 数据还显示, 在调查所涉期间,面对面社交和体育活动有所减少。可见,本句是对H段这两句的同义转述。题干中 activities involving direct contact with people对应原文中的 face- to- face socializing; were found to be on the decline是原文中 had declined的变化说法。

39.答案: D

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 In response to past critics, Twenge and her co- researchers stress they are not trying to prove。文章段落中涉及特文格和她的同事们回应批评者的内容出现在D段。该段第三、四句谈到, 当特文格用这些数据来说明屏幕使用时间效应时,一些评论者很快提出质疑,认为其听上去具有因果关系的说法其实是建立在相互关联的数据上的, 并且没有充分考虑其他潜在因素。紧接着最后一句, This time around“这一次”表示后面是对上述质疑的回应:特文格和她的团队强调他们并非试图证明这个原因,而是在评估其他潜在原因的合理性。文中causes as such指上文B、C段中论述的 the most. likely cause is the electronic riches we have given them, 即电子设备的使用导致年轻人的幸福感降低。可见,本句是对D段内容的概述。题干中 past critics是对原文D段第三、四句的概括; stress是原文 make a point of saying的同义转述。

40.答案: B

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 living standards went up和 economic depressions were largely averted。文章段落中提到生活水平上升和经济萧条得以避免的内容出现在B段。该段第二、三句提到,从20世纪60年代到21世纪初,美国人的平均幸福指数提高了,特别是年轻人。紧接着第四句指出,这反映出一个事实: 这几十年来,人们的总体生活水平在上升,全面战争或经济匮乏等大规模的社会创伤得以避免。将这三句话概括一下, 就是“20世纪最后几十年的美国,大规模的社会创伤得以避免, 生活水平提高, 导致人们幸福感提升”,本题是对此的同义转述。题干中的 In the last few decades of the 20th century对应原文的 from the 1960s into the early 2000s; living standards went up对应原文中的a climb in general standards of living; economic depressions were largely averted对应原文中的 avoidance of… economic deprivation。

41.答案: E

解析: 注意抓住题干关键词 doing homework。文章段落中提及做作业的内容出现在E段。该段第四、五句谈到,体育活动减少、与同龄人见面的次数减少会导致青少年幸福感降低, 阅读印刷媒体(报纸)的时间减少、做家庭作业的时间减少也会导致青少年幸福感降低,后者令人惊讶。最后一个发现似乎与另一个流行的假设相矛盾,即正是我们给学生的作业负担过重导致了所有的问题。可见,流行观点是“给学生的作业负担过重导致了所有的问题”,而研究人员发现的是,做家庭作业的时间减少会降低青少年的幸福感,那么可以得出结论:与流行观点相反,做作业可能会提高学生的幸福感。本句是对E段此处内容的合理推断。

42.答案: J

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 the researchers' new study has gone a step further。文章段落中谈及新研究更进一步的相关内容出现在J段。该段最后一句作者表明观点:这项新的研究确实比以前的研究更进一步,指出屏幕使用时间仍应该被认为是阻碍年轻人健康幸福的潜在障碍。也就是说,作者认为,在屏幕使用时间对年轻人幸福感的影响问题上,研究者们的新研究是进步的。本句是对原文的同义转述。

43.答案: F

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息extended screen time makes young people less happy。文章段落中谈到屏幕使用时间长短与年轻人快乐与否的关系的内容出现在F段。该段前半部分首先说到,在数字设备上花费时间很少的青少年幸福感最高,其幸福感甚至比那些从不使用这类设备的人还要高。紧接着在第四句指出,更长的屏幕使用时间明显与幸福感降低有关。可见,屏幕使用时间延长导致年轻人更加不快乐,本句是原文第四句的同义转述。题干中的 extended screen time对应原文中的 higher doses of screen time; less happy对应原文中的 lower happiness。

44.答案: I

解析: 注意抓住题干中的关键信息 economic inequality, family income和 affect people's wellbeing。文章段落中谈及经济不平等、家庭收入与人们幸福感的关系是在I段。该段第三句提到,他们发现了一些证据表明,收入不平等等一些粗略的衡量标准与幸福感的变化相关,但具有更直接影响的经济指标,如家庭收入和(使家庭陷入困境的)失业率,与幸福感没有任何关系。由此可知,影响人们幸福感的是经济上的不平等,而不是家庭收入。题干是对原文I段第三句的同义转述。题干中的 economic inequality和 family income均为原词复现。

45.答案: A

解析:注意题干说的是“人们普遍认为,屏幕使用时间过长是当今年轻人感到不幸福的原因”。这句话是在提出一种普遍观点,往往出现在文章开头部分, 结合题干关键词 today's young people可定位到A段。该段开头说现在的年轻人所享有令生活在过去的人羡慕的物质利益和法律保护。紧接着第五、六句转折指出,人们越来越怀疑这一切对我们的年轻人来说并非都是好的, 最流行的一个解释是,“屏幕使用时间”过长是罪魁祸首。后文借由特文格及其团队的研究来论述屏幕使用时间与幸福感的关系。可见, 此处是说人们普遍认为屏幕使用时间过长是当今年轻人感到不幸福的原因。本题对应A段。

46.定位:根据题干信息词milestone和development,可以把答案线索定位到第一段第三句。

解析:该句指出,一些专家甚至认为撒谎是一个像爬行和走路一样重要的成长阶段,因为它需要复杂的计划和注意力,以及为了操纵某个人而从此人的角度着情况的能力。选项A中的view complex situations from different angles是对原文中的see a situation from someone else's perspective的同义替换,故为正确答案。该句提到了标志着一个成长阶段的爬行和行走,但没有表明撒谎体现的能力比这两个能力更显著,因选项B无中生有,应排除。选项C、D 原文未提及,故均排除。

47.定位:根据题干信息词Harvard neuroscientist和takeswork,可以把答案线索定位到第二段。

解析:该段首句提到哈佛大学认知神经科学家说撒谎是需要费心思的。后面第四句提到在他的研究中,有一些受试者选择说谎,此时他们的前额颅腔壁控制网的活动增强,这正是高难度或复杂的思维过程的结果。第五句说这一情况表明,他们当时正在真相和不诚实之间做决定,并最终选择了后者。由此可知,撒谎沙及复杂的心理活动。选项D是对这两句话的总结概括,故为正确答案。选项A 、B原文未提及,故排除。本段第二句提到该研究中使用功能性核磁共振成像仪检测受试者的大脑,该仪器可以呈 现出血液流向大脑的活跃部位的情况。这体现的是撒谎时的大脑内部情况,而不是撒谎需要费心思的原因,因此选项C不正确。

48.定位:根据题干信息词tend tolie,可以把答案线索定位到第三段第二句。

解析:该句提到,当我们能够使谎言变得合理,当我们感到压力和疲劳,或者当我们看到别人不诚实时,我们更有可能撒谎。选项B 中的同伴压力属于本句提到的三种更易撒谎的情况中的第三种,即当我们看到别人不诚实时,故为正确答案。选项A、D原文未提及,属于无中生有,故排除。选项C 是强干扰项,第二段最后一句提到撒谎可能与无法抵抗诱惑有关,但并没有提及有强烈诱惑时人们更倾向于说谎,属于主观推断,故排除。

49.定位:根据题干信息词less ikely tolie,可以把答案线索定位到第三段第三句。

解析:该句提到,当我们受到道德方面的提醒或者认为别人在观察我们的时候,我们就不太可能撒谎。选项B中的under watchful eyes与原文中的others are watching为同义替换,故选项B正确。选项A是更易撇谎的情况,属于正反混滑,故排除。选项C、D原文未提及,故排除。

50.定位:根据题干信息词not get punished, 可以把答案线索定位到第三段第四句和第四段的最后两句。

解析:第三段第四句提到,当我们不惩罚撒谎的行为时,我们就增加了撒谎再次发生的可能性。第四段最后两句提到,当人们不用为不诚实承担后果时,谎言往往会变得更加耸人听闻。如果你给人们多个机会撒谎时,谎言会变得越来越大。题干中的not get punished对应原文中的faced no consequences,选项B中的tell bigger lies是原文lies which get bigger的同义转述,故选项B正确。其他三项在原文中均未提及,故排除。

51.定位:根据题干中的信息词“The Big One”,答案线索可以定位在文章第一段。

解析:文章第一段首句就提到“The Big One”,然后在第三句解释了什么是“The Big One”,即破折号后的内容:“这场不可避免的地震无疑将在著名的圣安德烈亚斯断层上造成各种破坏。”通过这句话我们可以得出,“The Big One” 就是一场巨大的自然灾害,故选项C为正确答案。选项A是“大地震”发生 的原因,而非地震本身,故排除。选项B、D显然错误。

52.定位:根据题干中的信息词Cascadia Rising dill,答案线索可以定位在文章第二段。

解析:文章第二段第二句提到:“ · 卡斯卡迪亚断层崛起’演习使当地居民和应急人员有机会演练在美国最危险且被低估的断层发生9.0级地震和海啸时的应对方法。”由此可知,“卡斯卡迪亚断层崛起”演 习的目的就是帮助人们应对可能会发生的地震和海啸。因此选项A为正确答案。选项B提到“迫在眉睫的灾害”,我们从文章最后一段知道,人们并不知道“大地震”何时会发生,不能用迫在眉睫来形容。选项C提到的“灾后生活”文中并没有相关信息支持,也排除。选项D 提到的“应对……后果”。在文中也没有相关信息支持,也排除。

53.定位:根据题干中的信息词a megathrust earthquake,答案线索可以定位在文章第四段。

解析:文章第四段第四句提到:“如果发生巨型逆冲断层滑动,所有的压力会释放出来,世界上最强烈的地震会发生。”由此可知,选项D 为正确答案。其他三个选项都是出题人根据第四段中出现的land plate boundaries where two plates converge设置的干扰项,故均排除。

54.定位:根据题干中的信息词officials,答案线索可以定位在文章第五、六段。

解析:文章第五段提到官员们担心发生另一次地震,因此让第一批应急反应者与公众一起进行演习。紧接着第六段提到:“并不是所有的伤亡都可以避免,但是通过跨地区、跨州,甚至跨国家边界的协调,官员们希望能够避免最坏的情况。”而最坏的情况即第六段开头提到的“如果发生9.0级地震,预计将有 数千人伤亡”。由此可知,避免最坏的情况即“在发生灾难时减少伤亡”,故选项B为正确答案。文中只提到通过各方协作减少伤亡,选项A只是演习的部分内容,不是目的,故排除。选项C与原文信息不符,原文提到的是减少人员伤亡,而不是财产损失,故排除。选项D是本题的强干扰项,因为第六段最后一句提到“在演习的同站上,官员们解释说,他们在这次演习中准备的报告将会影响到今后几年的灾害管理。”但是这句话只是说演习中准备的报告的作用,并非演习的最终目的,而且只提到灾害管理,并未提到应急管理,故也排除。

55.定位:本题的题干信息词是“The  Big One”, 信息比较笼统,而且全文都在围绕“大地震”进行叙述,所以考生很难精确定位答案线索所在段落。这时我们可以根据出题顺序和段落顺序基本一致的原则,将答案线索暂时定位在文章最后一段。

解析:文章最后一段提到:“对成千上万的卡斯卡迪亚居民来说,‘大地震’不是一个是否会发生的问题,而是一个何时会发生的问题。”由此可知选项C 是对该句话的同义转述,故为正确答案。其中原文中的not a question of if,only when和选项C中的a matter of time属于同义替换。选项A明显和原文信息“The Big One"isn't a question of if相反,故排除。文中没有提到“大地震”如何发生的信息,故排除选项B。文中也没有提到“大地震”是否一直困扰卡斯卡迪亚的居民的问题,因此选项D也排除。

用户搜索

疯狂英语 英语语法 新概念英语 走遍美国 四级听力 英语音标 英语入门 发音 美语 四级 新东方 七年级 赖世雄 zero是什么意思黄石市法院里英语学习交流群

  • 频道推荐
  • |
  • 全站推荐
  • 推荐下载
  • 网站推荐